nonverbal measures
Recently Published Documents


TOTAL DOCUMENTS

25
(FIVE YEARS 3)

H-INDEX

8
(FIVE YEARS 0)

2021 ◽  
Vol 21 ◽  
pp. 439-445
Author(s):  
Aan Widodo

The purpose of this research is to explain the nonverbal communication of legal counsel with the defendant in the conference room at the central Jakarta District Court. The concepts used were communication in trials and nonverbal communication. The study used a qualitative approach with descriptive research methods. Data is obtained by conducting interviews on the 6 informant, observations on 3 trials and literature studies. The results of this study showed that in the trial, legal counsel communicated with the defendant in an attempt to provide legal aid. One form of communication in legal aid is nonverbal communication. Nonverbal communication of legal counsel with defendants using gesture  "eye movement"  and  "sound Code". A Legal counsel with the defendant agreed nonverbal measures as a strategy for winning perceptions in the trial, which implicates the judge's "light" penalty.


Author(s):  
Marni Novick ◽  
Jay R. Lucker

Abstract Background Audiologists may choose to evaluate auditory temporal processing in assessing auditory processing abilities. Some may decide to use measures of nonverbal stimuli such as tonal or noise gap detection. Others may decide to use verbal measures such as time compressed sentences (TCS). Many may choose to use both. Purpose Since people typically come to audiologists for auditory processing testing complaining of problems processing verbal stimuli, the question arises whether measures of nonverbal stimuli provide evidence regarding a person's abilities to processing verbal stimuli. That is, are there significant correlations between measures of verbal stimuli and nonverbal stimuli that are used to evaluate auditory temporal processing? Research Design The present investigation is an exploratory study using file review of 104 people seen for routine auditory processing evaluations by the authors. Study Sample A file review was completed based on data from 104 people seen for auditory processing evaluations. Data Collection and Analyses The data from these 104 files were used to evaluate whether there are any correlations between verbal and nonverbal measures of auditory temporal processing. The verbal measure used was the TCS subtest of the SCAN-3 while the nonverbal measures included the gap detection screening from the SCAN-3 as well as the gaps-in-noise measures. Results from these tests were compared to determine whether any significant correlations were found based on results from Pearson product moment correlational analyses. Results None of the nonverbal measures were found to have a significant correlation with the TCS test findings based on the Pearson correlations used to analyze the data. Conclusion Results indicate that there are no significant correlations (relationships) between measures of auditory temporal processing using nonverbal stimuli versus verbal stimuli based on the tests used in the present investigation. These findings lead to a conclusion that tests using nonverbal stimuli are measuring different auditory processes than the measure of verbal stimuli used in the present investigation. Since people typically come complaining about understanding verbal input, it is concluded that audiologists should use some verbal measure of auditory temporal processing in their auditory processing test battery.


2019 ◽  
Vol 40 (04) ◽  
pp. 243-255
Author(s):  
Leah L. Kapa ◽  
Jessie A. Erikson

AbstractAlthough results vary across individual studies, a large body of evidence suggests that children with developmental language disorder (DLD) have domain-general deficits in executive function compared with peers with typically developing language. Poorer performance for children with DLD has been reported on verbal and nonverbal measures of sustained selective attention, working memory, inhibition, and shifting. However, examination of the variability of task scores among both children with and without DLD reveals a wide range of executive function performance for both groups. Additionally, using executive function scores to classify children into DLD versus typical groups results in classification accuracy that is not clinically useful. This evidence indicates that group-level differences in executive function abilities between children with and without DLD cannot be applied at the individual level. Many children with DLD appear to have intact executive function abilities, which undermines the possibility that poor executive functioning causes language deficits in this population. However, a substantial number of children with DLD also have executive function deficits, and, therefore, therapy approaches with this population should consider both their language and executive function abilities.


2017 ◽  
Vol 60 (8) ◽  
pp. 2170-2184 ◽  
Author(s):  
Leah L. Kapa ◽  
Elena Plante ◽  
Kevin Doubleday

Purpose The first goal of this research was to compare verbal and nonverbal executive function abilities between preschoolers with and without specific language impairment (SLI). The second goal was to assess the group differences on 4 executive function components in order to determine if the components may be hierarchically related as suggested within a developmental integrative framework of executive function. Method This study included 26 4- and 5-year-olds diagnosed with SLI and 26 typically developing age- and sex-matched peers. Participants were tested on verbal and nonverbal measures of sustained selective attention, working memory, inhibition, and shifting. Results The SLI group performed worse compared with typically developing children on both verbal and nonverbal measures of sustained selective attention and working memory, the verbal inhibition task, and the nonverbal shifting task. Comparisons of standardized group differences between executive function measures revealed a linear increase with the following order: working memory, inhibition, shifting, and sustained selective attention. Conclusion The pattern of results suggests that preschoolers with SLI have deficits in executive functioning compared with typical peers, and deficits are not limited to verbal tasks. A significant linear relationship between group differences across executive function components supports the possibility of a hierarchical relationship between executive function skills.


Assessment ◽  
2016 ◽  
Vol 25 (2) ◽  
pp. 173-182 ◽  
Author(s):  
Kasey G. Creswell ◽  
Michael A. Sayette ◽  
Jonathan W. Schooler ◽  
Aidan G. C. Wright ◽  
Laura E. Pacilio

We introduce a nonverbal “visceral” measure of hunger (i.e., squeezing a handheld dynamometer) and provide the first evidence of verbal overshadowing effects in this visceral domain. We presented 106 participants with popcorn and recorded their hunger levels in one of three conditions: (1) first report hunger using a traditional self-report rating scale (i.e., verbal measure) and then indicate hunger by squeezing a dynamometer (i.e., nonverbal measure), (2) first indicate hunger nonverbally and then indicate hunger verbally, or (3) indicate hunger only nonverbally. As hypothesized, nonverbal measures of hunger predicted subsequent eating behavior when they were uncontaminated by verbal measures—either because they preceded verbal measures of hunger or because they were the sole measure of hunger. Moreover, nonverbal measures of hunger were a better predictor of eating behavior than verbal measures. Implications of the study for communicating embodied experiences in a way that escapes the confines of symbolic representations are discussed.


2010 ◽  
Vol 48 (8) ◽  
pp. 884-888 ◽  
Author(s):  
Melissa Moore ◽  
Julie Aitken Schermer ◽  
Sampo V. Paunonen ◽  
Philip A. Vernon

PsycCRITIQUES ◽  
2005 ◽  
Vol 50 (25) ◽  
Author(s):  
Marianne LaFrance
Keyword(s):  

Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document