pupil diameter
Recently Published Documents


TOTAL DOCUMENTS

563
(FIVE YEARS 181)

H-INDEX

33
(FIVE YEARS 6)

2022 ◽  
Vol 12 ◽  
Author(s):  
Yuki Tsuji ◽  
So Kanazawa ◽  
Masami K. Yamaguchi

Pupil contagion is the phenomenon in which an observer’s pupil-diameter changes in response to another person’s pupil. Even chimpanzees and infants in early development stages show pupil contagion. This study investigated whether dynamic changes in pupil diameter would induce changes in infants’ pupil diameter. We also investigated pupil contagion in the context of different faces. We measured the pupil-diameter of 50 five- to six-month-old infants in response to changes in the pupil diameter (dilating/constricting) of upright and inverted faces. The results showed that (1) in the upright presentation condition, dilating the pupil diameter induced a change in the infants’ pupil diameter while constricting the pupil diameter did not induce a change, and (2) pupil contagion occurred only in the upright face presentation, and not in the inverted face presentation. These results indicate the face-inversion effect in infants’ pupil contagion.


2021 ◽  
Vol 11 (1) ◽  
pp. 177
Author(s):  
Yu Mizuno ◽  
Kazuyuki Hirooka ◽  
Yoshiaki Kiuchi

Recent advances in ocular aberrometry have revealed that ocular surgery increases ocular and corneal higher-order aberrations. This retrospective single-center study aimed to examine the effects of the overhanging bleb on corneal higher-order aberrations using a wavefront analyzer. We included 61 eyes from 50 patients with overhanging bleb after trabeculectomy with a fornix-based conjunctival flap using mitomycin C (overhanging bleb group) and 65 eyes from 54 glaucoma patients with no history of glaucoma surgery (control group). Corneal higher-order aberrations (total higher-order aberrations, coma aberrations, coma-like aberrations, spherical aberrations, and spherical-like aberrations) on a 4 mm pupil diameter were measured using the TOPCON KR-1W wavefront analyzer. Corneal coma aberrations were higher in the overhanging bleb group than in the control group (0.16 ± 0.13 μm and 0.10 ± 0.05 μm, respectively; p = 0.042). Corneal coma-like aberrations were also higher in the overhanging bleb group than in the control group (0.31 ± 0.32 μm and 0.16 ± 0.09 μm, respectively; p = 0.022). With an increasing ratio of cornea covered by the bleb to the entire cornea, all corneal higher-order aberrations increased except for corneal coma-like aberrations. Overhanging bleb after trabeculectomy with a fornix-based conjunctival flap using mitomycin C and its size influenced corneal higher-order aberrations.


2021 ◽  
Vol 15 ◽  
Author(s):  
Silvia L. Isabella ◽  
J. Allan Cheyne ◽  
Douglas Cheyne

Cognitive control of action is associated with conscious effort and is hypothesised to be reflected by increased frontal theta activity. However, the functional role of these increases in theta power, and how they contribute to cognitive control remains unknown. We conducted an MEG study to test the hypothesis that frontal theta oscillations interact with sensorimotor signals in order to produce controlled behaviour, and that the strength of these interactions will vary with the amount of control required. We measured neuromagnetic activity in 16 healthy adults performing a response inhibition (Go/Switch) task, known from previous work to modulate cognitive control requirements using hidden patterns of Go and Switch cues. Learning was confirmed by reduced reaction times (RT) to patterned compared to random Switch cues. Concurrent measures of pupil diameter revealed changes in subjective cognitive effort with stimulus probability, even in the absence of measurable behavioural differences, revealing instances of covert variations in cognitive effort. Significant theta oscillations were found in five frontal brain regions, with theta power in the right middle frontal and right premotor cortices parametrically increasing with cognitive effort. Similar increases in oscillatory power were also observed in motor cortical gamma, suggesting an interaction. Right middle frontal and right precentral theta activity predicted changes in pupil diameter across all experimental conditions, demonstrating a close relationship between frontal theta increases and cognitive control. Although no theta-gamma cross-frequency coupling was found, long-range theta phase coherence among the five significant sources between bilateral middle frontal, right inferior frontal, and bilateral premotor areas was found, thus providing a mechanism for the relay of cognitive control between frontal and motor areas via theta signalling. Furthermore, this provides the first evidence for the sensitivity of frontal theta oscillations to implicit motor learning and its effects on cognitive load. More generally these results present a possible a mechanism for this frontal theta network to coordinate response preparation, inhibition and execution.


2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Cesar A Sanchez Galeana

Abstract Purpose To assess the safety and efficacy of a novel compound with low -dose pilocarpine, brimonidine and oxymetazoline (PBO) in the near vision of a group of healthy presbyopic individuals. Design Prospective, consecutive, noncomparative, nonrandomized interventional study. Subjects Twenty-six healthy presbyopic patients presented from August to September 2021 to our clinic and were willing to participate in the study. Methods PBO compound was instilled in both eyes to assess its effect in near vision one hour after instillation.Main Outcome Measures Under corrected near visual acuity using the Jaeger notation, under corrected and corrected distance visual acuity, photopic and scotopic pupil diameter was registered before and one hour after instillation of the compound. Results Baseline binocular Jaeger notation was 5.86 +/-1.39 SD range (3-9). One hour after drop instillation, binocular Jaeger was 2.53 +/ 1.71 SD range (1 – 6) (p ≤ 0.0001) Lines gained 3.30 +/-1.40 SD range (0 – 6). Mean photopic pupilar diameter was 3.69 mm +/- 0.67 SD range (2.5 – 5 mm). Mean scotopic pupilar diameter was 4.61 mm +/- 0.78 SD range (3 – 6 mm). The mean photopic pupilar diameter was 2.27 mm +/-052 SD (1.5 – 3) (p ≤ 0.000). Scotopic pupilar diameter was 2.63 mm +/-0.53 SD range (2 – 3) (p ≤ 0.000). One patient reported no improvement in near vision. (3.8%). One patient reported dim vision rated 5 in a VAS (1 –10) (3.8%). Conclusion Low-dose pilocarpine, brimonidine, and oxymetazoline showed efficacy, improving near-distance vision one hour after instillation in a group of presbyopic patients, and safety showed few and transient secondary effects.


2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Nadia Paraskevoudi ◽  
Iria SanMiguel

Actions modulate sensory processing by attenuating responses to self- compared to externally-generated inputs, which is traditionally attributed to stimulus-specific motor predictions. Yet, suppression has been also found for stimuli merely coinciding with actions, pointing to unspecific processes that may be driven by neuromodulatory systems. Meanwhile, the differential processing for self-generated stimuli raises the possibility of producing effects also on memory for these stimuli, however, evidence remains mixed as to the direction of the effects. Here, we assessed the effects of actions on sensory processing and memory encoding of concomitant, but unpredictable sounds, using a combination of self-generation and memory recognition task concurrently with EEG and pupil recordings. At encoding, subjects performed button presses that half of the time generated a sound (motor-auditory; MA) and listened to passively presented sounds (auditory-only; A). At retrieval, two sounds were presented and participants had to respond which one was present before. We measured memory bias and memory performance by having sequences where either both or only one of the test sounds were presented at encoding, respectively. Results showed worse memory performance — but no differences in memory bias — and attenuated responses and larger pupil diameter for MA compared to A sounds. Critically, the larger the sensory attenuation and pupil diameter, the worse the memory performance for MA sounds. Nevertheless, sensory attenuation did not correlate with pupil dilation. Collectively, our findings suggest that sensory attenuation and neuromodulatory processes coexist during actions, and both relate to disrupted memory for concurrent, albeit unpredictable sounds.


2021 ◽  
Vol 10 (24) ◽  
pp. 5789
Author(s):  
Francisco Pérez-Bartolomé ◽  
Carlos Rocha-De-Lossada ◽  
José-María Sánchez-González ◽  
Silvia Feu-Basilio ◽  
Josep Torras-Sanvicens ◽  
...  

This study examines agreement between the devices Anterion® and Pentacam HR® used for corneal and pupil measurements in healthy eyes. The parameters compared between the two devices were: anterior Km (D), anterior K2 (D), anterior K1 (D), anterior K1 axis (°), anterior astigmatism (D), anterior K max (D), posterior Km (D), posterior K2 (D), posterior K1 (D), posterior K1 axis (°), posterior astigmatism (D), CCT (µm), thinnest point thickness (µm), thinnest point X-coordinate (mm), thinnest point Y-coordinate (mm), pupil diameter (mm), pupil center-corneal vertex distance (mm) (angle kappa), pupil centroid angle (°), pupil centroid X-coordinate (mm), and pupil centroid Y-coordinate (mm). The Student’s t test for independent samples identified significant differences (p < 0.005) between devices for the measurements anterior and posterior flat K axis, posterior flat K, steep K, and mean K. For these last three measurements, although significant, none of the differences were clinically relevant. Corneal power and thickness measurements except Kf axis showed excellent agreement between Anterion and Pentacam. In a clinical setting we would not recommend the interchangeable use of Pentacam and Anterion for measurement of pupil parameters.


2021 ◽  
Vol 12 ◽  
Author(s):  
Yang Liu ◽  
Weifeng Ma ◽  
Xiang Guo ◽  
Xuefen Lin ◽  
Chennan Wu ◽  
...  

In the present study, we tested the effectiveness of color coding on the programming learning of students who were learning from video lectures. Effectiveness was measured using multimodal physiological measures, combining eye tracking and electroencephalography (EEG). Using a between-subjects design, 42 university students were randomly assigned to two video lecture conditions (color-coded vs. grayscale). The participants’ eye tracking and EEG signals were recorded while watching the assigned video, and their learning performance was subsequently assessed. The results showed that the color-coded design was more beneficial than the grayscale design, as indicated by smaller pupil diameter, shorter fixation duration, higher EEG theta and alpha band power, lower EEG cognitive load, and better learning performance. The present findings have practical implications for designing slide-based programming learning video lectures; slides should highlight the format of the program code using color coding.


2021 ◽  
pp. 112067212110655
Author(s):  
Joaquim Fernández-Rosés ◽  
José Lamarca ◽  
David P. Piñero ◽  
Rafael I. Barraquer

Purpose To compare the accuracy of Sirius ray tracing software with the Barrett Universal II formula for intraocular lens power prediction in virgin eyes. Methods Retrospective case series including 86 eyes that have undergone uneventful cataract surgery with SN60WF implantation. The median absolute error, mean prediction error, variance, and the percentage of eyes within ± 0.25 D, ± 0.50 D, ± 0.75 D, and ± 1.00 D of the prediction error in refraction were calculated. The correlation of prediction error with different baseline parameters was investigated. Results No differences were found between the median absolute error of the Barrett Universal II formula (0.226 D) and the ray tracing software with different intraocular lens centerings; apex (0.331 D), limbus (0.345 D), and pupil (0.342 D) ( p = 0.084). The variance, from lowest to highest, was the Barrett Universal II (0.144 D2), ray tracing-limbus (0.285 D2), ray tracing-pupil (0.285 D2), and ray tracing-apex (0.287 D2) ( p = 0.027). The Barrett Universal II formula showed a higher percentage of eyes within ± 0.25 D (56.98%), ± 0.50 D (82.56%), and ± 0.75 D (93.02%) compared to ray tracing software ( p < 0.01). A significant correlation between the prediction error of the Barrett Universal II formula and corneal diameter (r = 0.322, p = 0.002) and pupil diameter (r = 0.230, p = 0.033) was found. Also, a positive correlation between the prediction error of Sirius ray tracing and axial length ( p < 0.001) and pupil diameter ( p = 0.01) was found. Conclusion There is a trend of the Barrett Universal II formula to be more accurate than Sirius ray tracing software for intraocular lens power calculation in virgin eyes. This should be confirmed in future prospective comparative studies.


2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Ignasi Cos ◽  
Gustavo Deco ◽  
Matthieu Gilson

Abstract Extensive research explains how pre-frontal cortical areas process explicit rewards, and how pre-motor and motor cortices are recipients of that processing to energize motor behaviour. However, the specifics of motor behaviour, decisions between actions and brain dynamics when driven by no explicit reward, remain poorly understood. Are patterns of decision and motor control altered wen performing under social pressure? Are the same brain regions that typically process explicit rewards also involved in this expression of motivation? To answer these questions, we designed a novel task of decision-making between precision reaches and manipulated motivation by means of social pressure, defined by the presence or absence of virtual partner of a higher/lower aiming skill than our participants. We assessed the overall influence of this manipulation by analysing movements, decisions, pupil dilation and electro-encephalography. We show that the presence of a partner consistently increased aiming accuracy along with pupil diameter, furthermore the more skilled the partner. Remarkably, increased accuracy is attained by faster movements, consistently with a vigour effect that breaches speed-accuracy trade-offs typical of motor adaptation. This implicated an ensemble of cortical sources including pre-frontal areas, concerned with the processing of reward, but also pre-motor and occipital sources, consistent with the nature of the task. Overall, these results strongly suggest the role of social pressure as a motivational drive, enabling an increase of both vigour and accuracy in a non-trivial fashion.


Author(s):  
Chinatsu Marumo ◽  
Tamami Nakano

Pupil diameter fluctuates in association with changes in brain states induced by the neuromodulator systems. However, it remains unclear how the neuromodulator systems control the activity of the iris sphincter (constrictor) and dilator muscles to change the pupil size. The present study compared temporal patterns of pupil dilation during movement when each muscle was pharmacologically manipulated in the human eye. When the iris sphincter muscle was blocked with tropicamide, the latency of pupil dilation was delayed and the magnitude of pupil dilation was reduced during movement. In contrast, when the iris dilator muscle was continuously stimulated with phenylephrine, the latency and magnitude of rapid pupil dilation did not differ from the untreated control eye, but sustained pupil dilation was reduced until the end of movement. These results suggest that the iris sphincter muscle, which is under the control of the parasympathetic pathway, is quickly modulated by the neuromodulator system and plays a major role in rapid pupil dilation. However, the iris dilator muscle receives signals from the neuromodulator system with a slow latency and is involved in maintaining sustained pupil dilation.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document