Binocular interaction and performance of visual tasks

2004 ◽  
Vol 24 (2) ◽  
pp. 82-90 ◽  
Author(s):  
Maria S. Justo ◽  
Maria A. Bermudez ◽  
Rogelio Perez ◽  
Francisco Gonzalez
1950 ◽  
Vol 2 (4) ◽  
pp. 176-181 ◽  
Author(s):  
D. C. Fraser

An experiment is described to test the relation between angle of display and performance in prolonged visual tasks, using the Clock Test in three positions of display surface, vertical, at an angle of 45 degrees to the horizontal, and horizontal. In every case the line of regard was kept at right-angles to the display surface. The results indicate that significantly fewer stimuli are missed in the vertical position of the display surface than in the other two. The deterioration observed in the second half-hour of the original Clock Test experiments was not found in the present experiment, and two possible reasons for this are discussed briefly.


Author(s):  
Klaus-Dieter Fröhner ◽  
Ze Li

Stability and instability are very important for the layout of real world processes concerning safety and health esp. when planned by scientists. The long‐term investigation of stability was carried out for the last ten years on the basis of the installation and the evaluation of an ergonomically designed outdoor illumination. In the depicted dynamic situation the lighting design influences directly visual discomfort and human performance and in the end stability and instability. The improvement of the adaptation of luminance and its influence on the visual tasks after the rearrangement are presented and discussed. The effective factors on the visual capability and performance of workers, work efficiency and potential accidents in the night shift, and furthermore the accelerators and barriers for the stability of the project are analysed and discussed.


Author(s):  
Adam M. Braly ◽  
Patricia R. DeLucia

Prior studies have shown that training with stroboscopic viewing improved performance on visual tasks, such as motion coherence thresholds, and performance on coincident anticipation tasks (Appelbaum, Schroeder, Cain, & Mitroff, 2011; Smith & Mitroff, 2012). In stroboscopic viewing, individuals wear occlusion goggles which present an intermittent view of the environment. It is assumed that training during “degraded” viewing will enhance subsequent performance during unimpaired viewing. We examined whether training with stroboscopic viewing can improve time-to-collision (TTC) judgments, which have importance in real-world tasks such as driving, using a prediction motion (PM) task (Schiff & Detwiler, 1979). The PM task is particularly well- suited for stroboscopic training because the task involves extrapolation of the object’s motion after it disappears (DeLucia & Liddell, 1998; Schiff & Oldak, 1990). In stroboscopic viewing, the object appears and then disappears, but does so repeatedly throughout the object’s approach. During periods of occlusion, observers putatively extrapolate the object’s motion. When the object reappears, observers get feedback on their extrapolation. Thus, they get feedback on their extrapolation throughout the object’s entire approach. Participants viewed computer simulations of an object that approached them and then disappeared. They judged TTC by pressing a button when they thought the object would hit them. Mean constant error of TTC judgments were compared among intervention conditions of stroboscopic training (5 minutes), continuous viewing (practice without feedback), and a control filler task. Performance was measured during four sessions—pre-test, intervention, immediately after intervention, and 10 minutes after intervention. Differences among the interventions were not significant, and judgment accuracy decreased across sessions. In contrast to Smith and Mitroff’s (2012) study of anticipatory timing of lateral motion, five minutes of stroboscopic training was not sufficient to improve TTC judgments of approaching objects. We considered several reasons why stroboscopic training did not improve TTC judgments. First, participants may not have mentally extrapolated the object’s motion when its view was occluded and thus did not benefit from its reappearance throughout the stroboscopic viewing. This seems unlikely, because research has shown that PM tasks involve motion extrapolation (DeLucia & Liddell, 1998). Second, the occlusion period may have been too short to allow observers to get feedback on their extrapolation of the object’s motion. We employed a strobe frequency of 4 Hz based on prior literature, but longer occlusion periods may be needed to see performance benefits and should be examined in future studies. Third, training that is more than 5 minutes may be required to show benefits for TTC judgments of approach motion (current study) than for lateral motion (Smith & Mitroff’s study). This may occur because the optical pattern is linear in lateral motion (the object’s change in position is the same throughout its trajectory) and non-linear in approach motion (the object’s change in optical size increases as it gets closer to the eye) and may result in the less accurate TTC judgments of approach compared to lateral motion (Schiff & Oldak, 1990). In conclusion, it is important to determine the conditions under which training can improve TTC judgments of approaching objects. If individuals can be trained to make more accurate TTC judgements, there are important implications for driver training programs. Drivers must anticipate the future position of vehicles that are around them when changing lanes, turning left, or overtaking vehicles, in traffic. Importantly, research has shown that observers have difficulty making these judgments and may misperceive the distance and speed of other vehicles (e.g., Caird & Hancock, 1994; Gray & Regan, 2005; Levulis, DeLucia, & Jupe, 2015). Driver training programs designed to improve observers’ abilities to judge TTC may help to reduce accidents.


Author(s):  
ZBIGNIEW LES ◽  
MAGDALENA LES

Understanding is based on a large number of highly varied abilities called intelligence that can be measured. In this paper understanding abilities of the shape understanding system (SUS) are tested based on the adoption of the intelligence tests. The SUS tests are formulated as the tasks given to the system and performance of SUS is compared with the human performance of these tasks. The main novelty of the presented method is that the process of understanding is related to the visual concept represented as a symbolic name of the possible classes of shape. The visual concept is one of the ingredients of the concept of the visual object (the phantom concept) that makes it possible to perform different tasks that are characteristic for the visual understanding. The presented results are part of the research aimed at developing the shape understanding method able to perform the complex visual tasks connected with visual thinking. The shape understanding method is implemented as the shape understanding system (SUS).


Author(s):  
Serene Mary J

The intent of this research is to explore how architecture can enlighten healing or provide spaces where healing can take place. The healing environments, make wellness centres less stressful and promote faster healing for patients and improve well-being for their families, as well as creating a pleasant, comfortable and safe work environment. Light impacts human health and performance by enabling performance of visual tasks, controlling the body’s circadian system, affecting mood and perception, and by enabling critical chemical reactions in the body. Natural light should be incorporated into lighting design in healthcare settings, not only because it is beneficial to patients and staff, but also because it is light delivered to people at no cost and in a form that most people prefer. Study involves comparing healthcare settings in warm and humid climatic zones and how day lighting plays an important role in healing. Appropriate day lighting thus impacts outcomes in healthcare settings by reducing depression among patients, decreasing length of stay in hospitals, improving sleep and circadian rhythm, lessening agitation among patients, easing pain, and improving adjustment to night-shift work among staff. The appropriate positioning and optimisation of windows/ openings in the workplace and access to daylight have been linked with increased satisfaction with the surrounding environment.


1973 ◽  
Vol 39 (6) ◽  
pp. 465-469 ◽  
Author(s):  
R. P. Waugh

The Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Abilities was administered to 166 second graders who were classified as auditory or visual learners on the basis of discrepancies in individual test profiles. Four controlled instructional procedures were presented in classroom settings. Two procedures were primarily auditory, and two primarily visual. The 5 percent of the subjects showing greatest preference for visual tests performed equally well on visual and auditory learning tasks. Auditory learners performed equally well on auditory and visual tasks.


Author(s):  
Jimmy L. Hatfield ◽  
David R. Soderquist

The performance of 32 subjects on 90-min. auditory and visual vigilance tasks were measured in conventional and theory of signal detection indices as a function of “coupling,” sense mode, and time on task. Order effects, correlations between sense modes, and possible effects of observing responses were also examined. Regardless of the coupling condition or sense mode involved, β increased while hits and false alarms decreased as time on task increased. There was no significant decline in d' over time on task for either sense mode or coupling conditions. In general, conventional measures of performance were similar for the auditory and visual tasks. Significant cross-modality correlations were obtained for false alarms, latency, and β values. No order effects were observed. The results are discussed in terms of expectancy theory and an observing response model.


1977 ◽  
Vol 21 (4) ◽  
pp. 322-325 ◽  
Author(s):  
Corwin A. Bennett ◽  
Anand Chitlangia ◽  
Abhay Pangrekar

Two experiments studied the relation of illumination level and practical task performance, and the effects of age. Performance improved with illumination and declined with age. The North American illumination standards seem adequate but not excessive.


Author(s):  
H. M. Thieringer

It has repeatedly been show that with conventional electron microscopes very fine electron probes can be produced, therefore allowing various micro-techniques such as micro recording, X-ray microanalysis and convergent beam diffraction. In this paper the function and performance of an SIEMENS ELMISKOP 101 used as a scanning transmission microscope (STEM) is described. This mode of operation has some advantages over the conventional transmission microscopy (CTEM) especially for the observation of thick specimen, in spite of somewhat longer image recording times.Fig.1 shows schematically the ray path and the additional electronics of an ELMISKOP 101 working as a STEM. With a point-cathode, and using condensor I and the objective lens as a demagnifying system, an electron probe with a half-width ob about 25 Å and a typical current of 5.10-11 amp at 100 kV can be obtained in the back focal plane of the objective lens.


Author(s):  
Huang Min ◽  
P.S. Flora ◽  
C.J. Harland ◽  
J.A. Venables

A cylindrical mirror analyser (CMA) has been built with a parallel recording detection system. It is being used for angular resolved electron spectroscopy (ARES) within a SEM. The CMA has been optimised for imaging applications; the inner cylinder contains a magnetically focused and scanned, 30kV, SEM electron-optical column. The CMA has a large inner radius (50.8mm) and a large collection solid angle (Ω > 1sterad). An energy resolution (ΔE/E) of 1-2% has been achieved. The design and performance of the combination SEM/CMA instrument has been described previously and the CMA and detector system has been used for low voltage electron spectroscopy. Here we discuss the use of the CMA for ARES and present some preliminary results.The CMA has been designed for an axis-to-ring focus and uses an annular type detector. This detector consists of a channel-plate/YAG/mirror assembly which is optically coupled to either a photomultiplier for spectroscopy or a TV camera for parallel detection.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document