Tactile Vigilance Is Stressful and Demanding

Author(s):  
Patricia R. DeLucia ◽  
Eric T. Greenlee

Objective The primary aims of the study were to replicate the vigilance decrement in the tactile modality, examine whether a decrease in sensitivity is associated with the decrement, and determine whether tactile vigilance is stressful and demanding. Background When people monitor occasional and unpredictable signals for sustained durations, they experience a decline in performance known as the vigilance decrement, which has important practical consequences. Prior studies of the vigilance decrement focused primarily on visual vigilance and, to a lesser degree, on auditory vigilance. There are relatively few studies of tactile vigilance. Method Participants monitored vibrotactile stimuli that were created from a tactor, for 40 min. Results Sensitivity declined, self-report ratings of distress increased, and ratings of task engagement decreased, during the vigil, and perceived workload was moderately high. Conclusion Monitoring tactile signals is demanding and stressful and results in a decrement in signal detection. Application Monitoring tactile signals may result in a decrement in tasks requiring discrimination, such as monitoring lane position with the use of rumble strips; these require discrimination between current road vibration and increased vibration when the car drifts out of its lane and crosses over the strip.

Author(s):  
Caroline R. Alikonis ◽  
Joel S. Warm ◽  
Gerald Matthews ◽  
William N. Dember ◽  
Edward M. Hitchcock ◽  
...  

Two models that seek to explain the high workload associated with vigilance tasks are the direct-cost and indirect-cost views. The former attributes the elevated workload to the high information-processing demand of the task; the latter attributes it to efforts to combat the boredom associated with monotonous vigilance tasks. A recent study by Hitchcock et al. (1999) provided support for the direct-cost view by showing that it is possible to lower the workload of vigilance through reductions in the information-processing load while leaving task-induced boredom unaffected. This study provides converging evidence for the direct-cost view: allowing observers to listen to a stress-reducing musical selection, Heart Zones, during a vigil lowered boredom while leaving the perceived workload of the task unaffected. The beneficial effect of the musical selection was limited to boredom; it had no impact upon post-vigil feelings of loss of task engagement and distress.


2017 ◽  
Vol 35 (6) ◽  
pp. 889-916 ◽  
Author(s):  
Ezgi Sakman ◽  
Nebi Sümer

This study examined whether the subliminal priming of threat and attachment figure availability interfere with cognitive attentional performance in conditions of uncertainty among individuals with differing attachment orientations. University students ( N = 225) first completed a scale to identify names of their significant attachment figures (WHOTO) and self-report measures of attachment anxiety and avoidance and were then administered a computerized signal detection task assessing their cognitive attentional performance under conditions of threat and attachment figure availability priming. Findings revealed that both attachment anxiety and avoidance posed risk factors for cognitive performance but in different patterns. While attachment avoidance made individuals more prone to errors in missing a signal that was present, attachment anxiety increased the error rate for false alarms. These findings are discussed in relation to previous work in the field and their implications for potential cultural differences.


Author(s):  
Victoria L. Claypoole ◽  
Alexis R. Dewar ◽  
Nicholas W. Fraulini ◽  
James L. Szalma

Vigilance, or sustained attention, refers to the capability of an individual to maintain attention to a stimulus over extended periods of time. Typically, vigilance tasks are associated with high levels of workload and stress, which manifests as less task engagement, and greater distress. Several factors have been shown to affect vigilance and its associated workload and stress (i.e. signal saliency, task difficulty, etc.). However, one factor that has been neglected in previous research on workload, stress, and anxiety is the presence of someone in a supervisory role during the task. Therefore, the purpose of the present investigation was to determine how the presence of a supervisory figure influenced perceived workload, stress, and anxiety that is associated with vigilance tasks. It was hypothesized that the presence of a supervisory figure would decrease workload and distress, and would increase the levels of anxiety, when compared to a non-supervisor.


2020 ◽  
Author(s):  
Ghasanfer Zulfiqar

The aim of this pilot study is to receive funding for a larger, nationwide conducted field study involving all 16 Polytechs in New Zealand. Perceived workload, actual work hours and and job satisfaction are to be measured initially for 5 Polytech tutors working in Applied business at NMIT Nelson using self-report questionnaires and a custom time-tracker app. A model has to be developed hypothesizing relationships between tutor characteristics; aspects of the teaching context; perceived workload; satisfaction with teaching; and four aspects of tutor stress: stress from administrative activities, time pressure, students and classroom conditions, and lack of rewards and recognition.


2020 ◽  
Author(s):  
Matthew Kyle Robison ◽  
Nash Unsworth ◽  
Gene Arnold Brewer

Across four experiments we examined the effects of goal-setting, feedback, and incentivizing manipulations on sustained attention. In addition to measuring task performance, we measured subjective attentional states and momentary feelings of motivation and alertness. Experiment 1 compared two specific goal conditions - one difficult and one easy - to a standard set of instructions. The specific goal conditions both reduced RTs and attenuated the vigilance decrement but did not impact task engagement (motivation or task-unrelated thoughts). Experiment 2 manipulated both goal-setting and feedback across conditions. The combination of a specific goal and feedback had strong effects on both task performance and task engagement. Additionally, feedback increased task engagement (higher motivation and fewer task-unrelated thoughts) regardless of whether or not it was paired with a specific goal. Experiment 3 examined the effect of pairing goals with a reward. Participants in one reward condition (time-based incentive) reported higher motivation, but did not show better task performance. Offering a cash incentive to meet a goal did not have an effect on any dependent variables. Finally, in an effort to examine whether more moderately-difficult goals might lead to optimal performance, Experiment 4 examined a broader range of goals. However we did not see an effect of a moderately-difficult goal on any of the dependent variables. None of the experimental manipulations were effective in mitigating or eliminating the vigilance decrement. We discuss the theoretical implications of the results with regard to goal-setting theory and theories of vigilance.


2018 ◽  
Author(s):  
M.T. Sherman ◽  
A.K. Seth ◽  
A.B Barrett

AbstractHow sure are we about what we know? Confidence, measured via self-report, is often interpreted as a subjective probabilistic estimate on having made a correct judgement. The neurocognitive mechanisms underlying the construction of confidence and the information incorporated into these judgements are of increasing interest. Investigating these mechanisms requires principled and practically applicable measures of confidence and metacognition. Unfortunately, current measures of confidence are subject to distortions from decision biases and task performance. Motivated by a recent signal-detection theoretic behavioural measure of metacognitive sensitivity, known as meta-ď, here we present a quantitative behavioural measure of confidence that is invariant to decision bias and task performance. This measure, which we call m-distance, captures in a principled way the propensity to report decisions with high (or low) confidence. Computational simulations demonstrate the robustness of m-distance to decision bias and task performance, as well as its behaviour under conditions of high and low metacognitive sensitivity and under dual-channel and hierarchical models of metacognition. The introduction of the m-distance measure will enhance systematic quantitative studies of the behavioural expression and neurocognitive basis of subjective confidence.


Author(s):  
Alexis R. Neigel ◽  
Daryn A. Dever ◽  
Victoria L. Claypoole ◽  
James L. Szalma

Objective: The goal of the present study is twofold: (1) demonstrate the importance of measuring and understanding the relationship between task engagement and vigilance performance, and (2) celebrate the work of Joel S. Warm and expand upon his previous research in two semantic vigilance paradigms. Background: The importance of measuring task engagement in cognitive and sensory vigilance tasks has been well documented. But to date, our understanding of the effects of task engagement on semantic vigilance performance is limited. Method: Seventy-three participants completed either a standard semantic vigilance task or a lure semantic vigilance task. Participants also completed subjective measures of workload and stress. Results: The results indicated that changes in task engagement are associated with correct detection performance. Changes in task engagement may be related to individual differences in the distress associated with performing semantic vigilance tasks. Conclusion: In line with the work of Warm and his colleagues (Dember, Warm, Bowers, & Lanzetta, 1984), participants who reported increased task engagement after the vigil outperformed their peers who noted decreased task engagement upon conclusion of the task. Participants reporting increases in engagement with the semantic vigilance tasks also reported significantly greater distress pretask, but not posttask. Instead, increases in postvigil distress were driven by the task to which participants were assigned, not task engagement. Application: The present study has several implications for applied settings that involve long duration semantic processing or semantic target identification. Such real-world tasks include aviation, cyber threat detection and analysis, driving, and reading.


Author(s):  
William S. Helton ◽  
Diana Fields ◽  
Joseph A. Thoreson

Stress is an important aspect of operational settings. This article presents a study providing further psychometric and validation evidence of a short multidimensional self-report measure of stress state, the Short Stress State Questionnaire (SSSQ; Helton, 2004) based on the Dundee Stress State Questionnaire (DSSQ; Matthews et al., 1999, 2002). Participants filled out the SSSQ twice daily for 70 days, once in the morning and once at night. They also reported daily cognitive slipsfailures. Chain-P Factor Analyses of the individual items for both pre and post assessments were conducted and the relationships between the SSSQ factors and daily self-reported cognitive slipsfailures were examined. The factor analyses, as previously indicated by Helton (2004), differentiated three aspects of subjective stress: Task Engagement, Distress, and Worry. Daily post-Distress and post-Worry correlated moderately with cognitive slips. The 24-item SSSQ appears to be a reliable measure of daily stress state, potentially useful in naturalistic studies.


Author(s):  
Victoria L. Claypoole ◽  
Daryn Denver ◽  
Kody Denues ◽  
James L. Szalma

Vigilance refers to the ability of an observer to maintain attention over extended periods of time, and to respond to critical signals that occur (Davies & Parasuraman, 1982). Vigilance has been examined since the late 1940s when anecdotal evidence suggested that naval operators missed more critical signals (signifying the presence of enemy submarines) on their displays the longer they were on watch. Mackworth (1948) confirmed this anecdotal evidence; vigilance declined quickly as the task progressed and more critical signals were missed the longer a participant stayed on task (i.e. the vigilance decrement). Although the research on vigilance originated almost 70 years ago, it continues to be of importance to human factors psychologists (Reinerman-Jones, Matthews, Langheim, & Warm, 2011). Recently, research has looked towards other domains of psychology (i.e. social psychology) for variables that may also be motivational factors to attenuate the vigilance decrement. One such factor is the construct of social facilitation. The study of social facilitation involves investigating how social presence affects an individual’s performance (Guerin & Innes, 1982). Typically, the social presence of another individual enhances performance on a simple, or a well-learned, task, while it impairs performance on a complex, or an unfamiliar task (Bond & Titus, 1983). The research on social facilitation and vigilance is limited. To date, only a handful of studies have indicated that social presence may influence task performance on a vigil (Bergum & Lehr, 1963; Claypoole & Szalma, 2017; Funke et al., 2016; Putz, 1975; Yu & Wu, 2015). The results of these experiments have been inconsistent. For example, the type of social presence utilized varies from co-actors (Funke et al., 2016) to merely present individuals (Yu & Wu, 2015) to evaluative observers (Claypoole & Szalma, 2017; Putz, 1975). Moreover, the type of performance affected also ranges from proportion of hits (Bergum & Lehr, 1963) to response time (Yu & Wu, 2015) to proportion of false alarms (Claypoole & Szalma, 2017). These inconsistencies may be the result of the type of task used. The construct of social facilitation relies on the operationalization of improved performance on a simple, or a well-learned, task and impaired performance on a complex, or an unfamiliar task (Bond & Titus, 1983; Zajonc, 1965). Therefore, it is possible that some of the previous tasks were “too difficult” for social facilitation effects to occur on all areas of performance. Therefore, it is necessary to statistically establish whether a task is “easy” or “difficult” for use in research on social facilitation and vigilance. Previous research has suggested that one way to manipulate task complexity is through the use of event rate (Warm & Jerison, 1984). Event rate refers to the presence of background signals typically separated into two categories, complex and simple, where the number of events per time unit in a complex event rate is greater than the number of events in a simple event rate (Stearman & Durso, 2016). With the occurrence of a faster, more complex event rate, performance tends to diminish (Warm & Jerison, 1984). This supports an inverse relationship between vigilance and event rate where the hit rate of a critical signal diminishes with the increase in complexity of the event rate (Guralnick, 1973). With a more complex event rate, greater “capacity” is needed to successfully complete the task (Parasuraman & Giambra, 1991). With the increased presentation of neutral targets, the quality of attention tends to diminish over time (Warm & Jerison, 1984). Researchers investigating vigilance, and potential factors that may alleviate the decrement, may use event rate to determine performance on unfamiliar tasks with ranging difficulties. The purpose of the present study is to statistically establish a difference in task difficulty between two versions of the same task in order to provide additional evidence that event rate may be used as a manipulation of task difficulty. Event rate has been previously established as a manipulation of “relative task difficulty” because it requires different amounts of information processing (Warm & Jerrsion, 1984). Difficulty, in this instance, is quantified as the amount of information processing required to complete a task. If one task has more information to process, it is thus considered to be more difficult. As such, if the versions do differ in difficulty, then significant differences in performance and perceived workload should be observed. The present study utilized two, 24-minute cognitive tasks in order to study the effects of event rate on vigilance. Across both conditions, typical vigilance effects were observed. The decrement manifested as a significant decrease in proportion of correct detections as a function of time on watch. Additionally, response time as increased across both conditions as a function of period on watch. Typical vigilance effects were also observed in sensitivity and response bias, such that there was a significant decrease in sensitivity across both conditions as a function of period of watch and there was a significant increase in response bias across conditions as a function of period of watch. These results indicate that the task parameters utilized in the present study are appropriate for examining the effects of vigilance. Utilizing the NASA-TLX to measure perceived workload, the results indicated high global workload across both conditions, a typical trend in vigilance research. Furthermore, the results indicated that those in the “more difficult” version reported higher levels of overall workload, mental demand, and effected performance than those in the “easy” version. Overall, these results provide further evidence that event rate can be used as a manipulation to change relatively task difficulty.


Author(s):  
Michel Loeb

In a recent article, Craig (1977) has considered the question, first attacked by Broadbent and Gregory (1963), as to the applicability of signal detection theory (SDT) to the vigilance situation, and he has discussed the use of group data by Loeb and Binford (1964) in approaching this same question. In a still more recent article by Craig (1978) it is argued that a probability-matching model is appropriate for explaining the “vigilance decrement” with time on task. Here it is agreed that to a degree SDT may be applied to vigilance and that some matching of response frequency to signal frequency may occur, but it is argued that there are changes within and across sessions which may not be explained in these terms and which may involve learning about the differential characteristics of signal and non-signal stimuli.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document