Measuring the Infinite Optical Thickness of Dentine Porcelain of the IPS E.max

2011 ◽  
Vol 492 ◽  
pp. 349-353 ◽  
Author(s):  
Long Quan Shao ◽  
Lin Lin Wang ◽  
Qi Liu ◽  
Bin Deng ◽  
Ning Wen

Dentine porcelain disc specimens 13.0 mm in diameter with thicknesses of 1.0, 1.5, 2.0, 2.5, 3.0, 3.5, 4.0, 4.5, 5.0, 5.5, and 6.0 mm, were made using IPS E.max color systems B, C, and D and their chromatic aberration values (ΔE) under the standard black background and the standard white background were measured using a Minolta CM-5 spectrophotometer. Then, the cubic regression formula of best fit was used to calculate the thicknesses of dentine porcelains of various color numbers at a ΔE of 1.0 and 1.5. The determination coefficient of the regression formula of color system B was 0.990–0.995, that of color system C was 0.989–0.997, and that of D was 0.993–0.998; and R2is close to 1. The results show that the regression formulas of the 11 thickness groups have good fit. The infinite optical thickness values of the IPS E.max dentine porcelains were as follows: for B1–B4, 3.263–2.711 (ΔE = 1.0) and 2.910–2.493 (ΔE = 1.5); for color system C, 2.727–2.611 (ΔE = 1.0) and 2.473–3.365 (ΔE = 1.5); and for color system D: 2.928–3.694 (ΔE = 1.0) and 2.656–2.458 (ΔE = 1.5). When the chromatic aberration tolerance is 1.5, the thickness of dentine porcelain required to reach the infinite optical thickness is less than the numerical value at ΔE = 1.0. As the thickness increases, the influence of the background diminishes and the specimen looks truer to its inherent color. The results prove that thickness of dentine porcelain positively correlates with its masking ability and that its thickness must exceed its infinite optical thickness so that its color will not be influenced by the background color.

2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
M Saegusa ◽  
H Kurokawa ◽  
N Takahashi ◽  
T Takamizawa ◽  
R Ishii ◽  
...  

Clinical Relevance Although excellent color-matching ability was confirmed for the structural colored resin composite Omnichroma, care should be taken to ensure that the appropriate chromatic color conditions for restoration are achieved in the oral environment. SUMMARY Purpose: The present study evaluated the color-matching ability of a structural colored resin composite to compare it with resin composites employing pigments. Methods and Materials: A structural colored resin composite (Omnichroma [OMC]), a supranano-filled resin composite (Estelite Σ Quick [ELQ]), and a nano-filled resin composite (Filtek Supreme Ultra [FSU]) were used. Each resin composite was packed into a Teflon mold and pressed down with a clear strip under a glass slide. The specimens were light irradiated through the slide with a light-emitting diode curing unit. The thickness of the specimens (n=6) was measured with a digital caliper before being transferred to distilled water and stored at 37°C for 24 hours. The measurements of the optical characteristics of the specimens on a black-and-white background were performed using a spectrophotometer. D65 (CIE D65) was used as a light source for the spectrophotometer. Measurements were repeated three times for each specimen under each color-measurement condition, and average values for three same-shade specimens were calculated. One-way analysis of variance and Tukey post hoc tests were used (α=0.05). To determine its ability to match the color of artificial teeth, each shade of resin composite was placed in a cavity before performing color measurements. Using a spectrophotometer (CMS-35F S/C) with a flexible sensor, L*, a*, and b* values were obtained. Results: The spectral reflectance curve of OMC showed that it reflected light wavelengths from 430-700 nm regardless of the background color and thickness of the specimens. The percentage of reflectance of ELQ decreased near wavelengths of 550-580 nm. Regarding the influence of background color on CIE L*, a*, b* values, the L* level showed significantly higher values for all tested materials with white backgrounds, and OMC was most affected by the difference in background color. However, a* values of ELQ and FSU were significantly higher with a black background than with a white background, and OMC showed a significantly higher value with a white background than with a black background. The b* values were higher with a white background than with a black background and were significantly higher for all three products, and these tendencies were much greater for ELQ and FSU. Conclusions: The ability of OMC to match the color of artificial teeth showed acceptable color compatibility, regardless of the shade of the artificial teeth and the depth of the cavity. However, ELQ and FSU showed reduced color compatibility, especially for a cavity depth of 3.0 mm. Excellent color matching ability was confirmed for the structural colored resin composite OMC, resulting in reduced color differences and therefore improving the esthetic appearance of the restoration, simplifying shade matching, and compensating for any color mismatch.


2021 ◽  
Vol 28 (2) ◽  
pp. 33-43
Author(s):  
Salahaddin Baper ◽  
Husein Husein ◽  
Sazgar Salim

Color has a crucial impact on students’ perception. It encourages the learning atmosphere to be affiliated with the anticipated learning outcomes. The purpose of this study is to investigate the impacts of contextual colors on student’s perception of interior spaces and to validate previous related studies that emphasize on colors as a media to convey meanings that affect behaviors and students’ perception as well. Accordingly, the objective of this study is to determine and validate previous related studies regarding the impact of background color on student’s perception. Meanwhile the study evaluates the previous relevant models in order to validate or reject previous assumptions. The study concentrates on quantitative survey method using in depth questionnaire to determine student’s perception at UHDUniversity of Human Development, Sulaimani. Seven Colors from Munsell color system (yellow, green, blue, purple, white, and black, grey) are applied in the test in order to indicate the students’ perceptual status. The results show that cool colors like white, green and blue are mostly recommended for educational spaces. These colors affected positively the perception of respondents and add feeling of hope, curiosity and satisfaction to the group. In contrast dark colors (black and grey) are indicated as colors of worry and fear. The findings demonstrate that environments colors play an important effect on student’s perception. The study concluded that colors affect the behavior, performance and mood of the space users.


2002 ◽  
Vol 205 (16) ◽  
pp. 2535-2543 ◽  
Author(s):  
Erik Höglund ◽  
Paul H. M. Balm ◽  
Svante Winberg

SUMMARYIn salmonid fish, a darker skin colour has been suggested to signal social subordination. Substratum colour is another factor affecting skin pigmentation in fish; in the present experiment, juvenile Arctic charr (Salvelinus alpinus) were acclimated and allowed to interact in pairs for 5 days on a pale or dark background colour. Skin darkness was quantified prior to and following social interaction. Furthermore, agonistic behaviour and skin darkness were quantified, together with plasma levels of cortisol,adrenocorticotropin (ACTH) and α-melanocyte-stimulating hormone(α-MSH), and brain levels of monoamines and monoamine metabolites. The results show that fish interacting on a white background were more aggressive than those interacting on a black background. Social subordination resulted in skin darkening in fish kept on a white background, but not in fish kept on a black background. Furthermore, subordinate fish on a white background showed an elevation of brain norepinephric activity, an effect not seen in subordinate fish on a black background. Subordinate fish on both white and black backgrounds showed a similar activation of the brain serotonergic system and the hypothalamic—pituitary—interrenal axis. These results support the suggestion that skin darkening in subordinates acts as a social signal announcing social submission.


In a previous contribution (Hogben and Slome, 1931) evidence was brought forward to show that the white background response does not depend on the same mechanism of coordination as the black background response, which is produced by reflex liberation of a hormone (“B” substance) of the pars intermedia in the pituitary gland; and experiments pointing to the existence of another internal secretion (“W” substance), connected directly or indirectly with the activity of the pars tuberalis, were described. The existence of separate receptor components of the retina controlling the two systems was left for subsequent enquiry. Of two possible hypotheses concerning the nature of the receptive mechanism, the most likely one is illustrated diagrammatically in fig. 1. In normal situations, when an animal is illuminated on a black background, light can only fall on the floor of the retina. If it is aquatic, the maximum divergence of any two rays which strike the eye is twice the critical angle for air and water, so that in the absence of reflexion of rays from sur­rounding objects below the surface of separation all rays will presumably be brought to a sharp focus in shallow water. There are thus three distinct possibilities which arise from the way in which the animal is illuminated if, as in Xenopus , the eyes are situated on the top of the head: ( a ) in darkness no part of the retina is stimulated, the same being true of the eyeless animal; ( b ) when the animal is exposed to a black background only a sharply localized region of the retina is stimulated; ( c ) when the animal is exposed to a white background the whole of the retina is illuminated owing to the scattering of rays in all directions from the surroundings. For convenience of description the usual black back­ground situation will be described hereafter as one in which only the “floor” of the retina is stimulated, and the white background situation as one in which the floor and the “ periphery ” of the retina are both stimulated together. If then, the receptor elements of the floor and periphery initiate different systems of reflex arcs the phenomena of the background response in Amphibia and Reptiles may be interpreted as follows. In Reptiles we may suppose that stimulation of floor reflexly excites the melanophores to expand, while stimulation of peripheral photoreceptors excites them to contract, being presumably prepotent in the final common path. In Amphibia two alternatives may be con­sidered: ( a ) that floor elements reflexly excite liberation of “B” and that peripheral photoreceptors, being prepotent, reflexly inhibit libera­tion of “B"; ( b ) that floor elements reflexly excite liberation of “B”, and peripheral photoreceptors reflexly excite production of the antago­nistic substance “W” in quantity sufficient to over-ride the effect of “B”. The crucial test of the truth of the general hypothesis that the floor and peripheral elements of the retina initiate different processes of coordina­tion was suggested by Keeble and Gamble (1904-6) in their experiments on Crustacea. If it is true, a normal animal illuminated from below in a black tank with a white top should react in exactly the same way as a normal animal when illuminated in a black tank from above. On the other hand, a normal animal illuminated from below in a black tank with a black top should react like an eyeless animal in the same situation, because the floor elements would not be subject to stimulation. The experiment may be varied as indicated below. In order to obtain signi­ficant results two classes of precautions must be carefully observed. One is that the physical dimensions of the tank must not exceed certain limits, since the maximal divergence of two rays is rigidly fixed when an animal is illuminated from above. The other is that there must be no air-water interface to permit reflexion of the incident rays downwards. Aside from the fact that no bubbles must be allowed to collect, this condition presents a practical difficulty if the animal has to come to the surface to breathe.


Author(s):  
Jessica L King ◽  
Connor Simper ◽  
Jacob Razzouk ◽  
Julie W Merten

Abstract Introduction Advertisement warnings are often overlooked, which reduces the opportunity for risk communication. Methods We used Prolific to survey 1,131 young adults (18-35) who currently used e-cigarettes or tobacco products. We randomized participants to one of four warning conditions: black text on white background (BW), white on black (WB), black on yellow (BY), and yellow on black (YB). We examined associations between condition and attention, recall, ad appeal, perceived message effectiveness (PME), and intentions to use e-cigarettes using chi-square and ANOVA where appropriate. We conducted logistic regressions by condition for attention and recall controlling for demographics and tobacco use. Results The warning was selected as the most attention-capturing area of the advertisement more often by those exposed to yellow warnings than white (59.9% versus 46.8%), even after controlling for demographics and tobacco use (p<.05). Recall was greater among those exposed to yellow warnings than white (44.2% vs. 37.3%), which held in controlled models. There were no significant differences between yellow and white warnings for ad appeals, PME, or intentions to use. In subanalyses, WB warnings generated higher PME (10.1 vs. 9.5) and lower intentions to use e-cigarettes (3.0 vs. 3.3) than BW (each p<.05). Conclusions Yellow warning color increases attention and recall of the warning, but this increase in attention did not translate to differences in downstream effects. Among currently mandated warning variations, the white text on black background warning appears more effective than the black on white. Future research should examine whether differences translate to behavior change. Implications We tested color variations of the FDA-mandated nicotine text warning on e-cigarette advertisements. Yellow variations (yellow text on black background and black on yellow) better captured attention and increased warning recall compared to the mandated black and white warnings. Among the FDA-mandated black on white and white on black warnings, the white on black variation appears more effective, generating higher perceived message effectiveness and lower intentions to use e-cigarettes. Given the difficulty in implementing pictorial warnings in the US, color might represent an alternative to improve warning effectiveness. Findings may also be applicable to those designing tobacco-related health communications.


1983 ◽  
Vol 27 (1) ◽  
pp. 46-50 ◽  
Author(s):  
Jack D. Grimes

Flicker was studied as perceived on a raster-scan CRT-based computer terminal in a typical use environment. The subject of the research was the relationship between the perception of flicker and the kind of spatial image presented. Both traditional images (filled white disks or squares) and images consisting of text and line drawings, similar in content to typical graphic computer terminal displays, were used in the study. This study measured the flicker perception of eight people. The most significant finding from this study was that CFF thresholds for some images presented on CRT displays in a typical use setting, differed significantly from more traditional images used in most CFF studies reported in the literature. For the eight images tested, the two which exhibited significantly higher CFF thresholds were lines or text on a black background (patterns 3 and 4). Therefore, an important factor to be considered in the prediction of the flicker characteristics of a 60 Hertz, noninterlaced CRT display in conditions similar to those of this study is the choice of normal video (black background) or reverse video format. The white background video format is likely to flicker when the display luminance is above 20 ft-L while the black background video format threshold was about 80 ft-L for both patterns 3 and 4. Alternatively, this luminance difference corresponds to about a 10 Hertz shift in CFF. In this study, the choice of video format alone, resulted in a luminance difference of 60 ft-L at CFF, or alternatively, resulted in a 10 Hertz shift in CFF at a particular luminance, for images normally found on actual CRT displays.


1970 ◽  
Vol 19 (4) ◽  
pp. 2945-2953
Author(s):  
Samuel Bert Boadi-Kusi ◽  
Michael Agyemang Kwarteng ◽  
Emmanuel Asubonteng

Aim: To design and validate a MNREAD Asante Twi version reading chart, to aid in the assessment of near visual acuity of natives Ghanaians.Methods: Cross-sectional and experimental designs were employed in phases I and II respectively of this study. The chart was developed using 20 transited pupils in class four in the Kumasi metropolis in phase I and validated in a clinical setting at the Manhyia District Hospital, Kumasi, using students in phase II.Results: A total of 100 participants (mean age; 22.19 ± 1.61 years) were involved in the second phase of this study. A Pearson product-moment correlation coefficient was computed to assess the relationship between MNREAD Asante Twi charts logMAR scores in both eyes. The MNREAD-Asante Twi acuity charts had very strong correlations (r = 0.94, p < 0.001) for MNREAD set 1A (black on white background) scores and MNREAD set 2A (black on white background) for acuity scores. Correlation between MNREAD set 1B (white on black background) scores and MNREAD set 2B (white on black background) scores was r = 0.95, p < 0.001.Conclusion: MNREAD Asante Twi reading chart will enhance the measurement of near visual function in native Ghanaians.Keywords: Asante Twi, MNREAD, Visual Acuity, Near chart, Ghana.


Neil (1939) has described the chromatic response of normal and blinded eels to various condition of illumination. From the times taken to equilibrate when passing from white to black “background” and vice versa with overhead illumination, he concluded that co-ordination is humoral. The time taken to equilibrate after transference to darkness from an illuminated white background or vice versa implies that control is bihumoral. Apart form Neill's work and an early comment by Petersen the chromatic behaviour of the eel has engaged little attention. Lode (1890) described contraction of the melanophores after faradic stimulation of the cord. Odiorne (1933) found that injection of posterior lobe pituitary extract caused expansion of the melanophores. The present communication deals with experiments designed to elucidate the mechanism of co-ordination more fully. It includes observations on ( a ) modification of normal chromatic behaviour by total or partial hypophysectomy; ( b ) effect of total and partial hypophysectomy on tolerance to pituitary extracts; ( c ) influence of nervous stimulation in the presence or absence of an intact blood supply. They are based chiefly on the behaviour of the dermal melanophores for recording which the melanophore index ( μ ) of Hogben and Slome (1931) is used throughout.


Data in Brief ◽  
2017 ◽  
Vol 14 ◽  
pp. 724-729 ◽  
Author(s):  
Kanta Mizusawa ◽  
Yutaka Yamamura ◽  
Satoshi Kasagi ◽  
José Miguel Cerdá-Reverter ◽  
Akiyoshi Takahashi

2009 ◽  
Vol 55 (4) ◽  
pp. 359-367 ◽  
Author(s):  
Ori Segev

The threat-sensitivity hypothesis assumes individuals should demonstrate flexibility in response to perceived predation risk and vary the intensity of anti-predator responses in concert with perceived risk of predation. Substrate color matching is adaptive as it enables organisms to become less conspicuous to both their prey and predators. I hypothesized that newborn fire salamander (Salamandra infraimmaculata) larvae will respond fast through physiological color change to contrasting backgrounds, becoming lighter against a white background and darker against a black background. Additionally, in accordance with the threat-sensitivity hypothesis, I expected a background color x predator interaction—i.e., that predator presence will further enhance the focal larvae color-matching response. To explicitly test these hypotheses I conducted a replicated outdoor mesocosm experiment. I used a two-by-two factorial design: pools of black or white background color crossed with the presence or absence of a larger cannibalistic conspecific. Digital photos of the focal larvae's dorsal view revealed that larval brightness and chroma changed accordingly against the contrasting black and white backgrounds to increase background matching. Although not statistically significant, larvae tended to show a stronger color-change response towards enhanced background matching in the presence of the free predator. Larval survival was strongly reduced in the presence of the larger conspecific, with no apparent effect of background color. This study demonstrates that Salamandra larvae are capable of environmentally induced physiological color change and highlights the need for further investigation into the interplay between threat intensity, mechanisms of risk assessment, and physiological antipredator responses.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document