orientation experiment
Recently Published Documents


TOTAL DOCUMENTS

15
(FIVE YEARS 7)

H-INDEX

5
(FIVE YEARS 1)

2022 ◽  
Vol 2149 (1) ◽  
pp. 012014
Author(s):  
J Christinck ◽  
B Rodiek ◽  
M López ◽  
H Georgieva ◽  
H Hofer ◽  
...  

Abstract We report on the characterization of the angular-dependent emission of two different single-photon emitters based on nitrogen-vacancy centers in nanodiamond and on core-shell CdSe/CdS quantum dot nanoparticles. The emitters were characterized in a confocal microscope setup by spectroscopy and Hanbury-Brown and Twiss interferometry. The angular-dependent emission is measured using a back focal plane imaging technique. A theoretical model of the angular emission patterns of the 2D dipoles of the emitters is developed to determine their orientation. Experiment and model agree well with each other.


2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Hui Huang ◽  
Yangming Zhang ◽  
Sheng Li

Perceptual training of multiple tasks suffers from interference between the trained tasks. Here, we conducted four psychophysical experiments with separate groups of participants to investigate the possibility of preventing the interference in short-term perceptual training. We trained the participants to detect two orientations of Gabor stimuli in two adjacent days at the same retinal location and examined the interference of training effects between the two orientations. The results showed significant retroactive interference from the second orientation to the first orientation (Experiments 1 and 2). Introducing a 6-hour interval between the pre-test and training of the second orientation did not eliminate the interference effect, excluding the interpretation of disrupted reconsolidation as the pre-test of the second orientation may reactivate and destabilize the representation of the first orientation (Experiment 3). Finally, the training of the two orientations was accompanied by fixations in two colors, each served as a contextual cue for one orientation. The results showed that the retroactive interference was not evident after introducing these passively perceived contextual cues (Experiment 4). Our findings suggest that the retroactive interference effect in short-term perceptual training of orientation detection tasks was likely the result of higher-level factors such as shared contextual cues embedded in the tasks. The effect of multiple perceptual training could be facilitated by associating the trained tasks with different contextual cues.


2021 ◽  
Vol 13 (9) ◽  
pp. 1812-1819
Author(s):  
Na-Na Yang ◽  
Hao-Rui Liu ◽  
Ning Mi ◽  
Qi Zhou ◽  
Li-Qun He ◽  
...  

Stereolithography (SLA)-manufactured parts behave with anisotropic properties due to the varying interface orientations generated by the layer-based manufacturing process. Part build orientation is a very important factor of anisotropic mechanical properties. In this paper, the build orientation experiment was designed to study the anisotropic behaviour of the mechanical properties of the SLA parts based on the orientation relationship between the force and the layer. The results show that there are obvious brittle characteristics on the fracture surface of the specimens and microcracks perpendicular to the direction of the layer distributed on the side of the fracture. The mechanical properties under brittle fracture have different degrees of sensitivity to the build orientation. Among all the build orientations, whether a specimen is built flat or on an edge shows obvious difference in tensile strength, and the relative range distribution reaches 35%. The changes in elastic modulus and the elongation at break are the most obvious in different angles relative to the XY plane, and the relative range distribution reaches 62% and 56% respectively. In all the build orientations designed, the tensile strength is the largest when it is placed on the edge at 0° with Y-axis in the XY plane, the elastic modulus is the largest when it was placed vertically, and the elongation at break is the largest when it is placed flat at 45° with Y-axis in the XY plane.


PLoS ONE ◽  
2021 ◽  
Vol 16 (7) ◽  
pp. e0254361
Author(s):  
Alexis D. J. Makin ◽  
John Tyson-Carr ◽  
Yiovanna Derpsch ◽  
Giulia Rampone ◽  
Marco Bertamini

An Event Related Potential (ERP) component called the Sustained Posterior Negativity (SPN) is generated by regular visual patterns (e.g. vertical reflectional symmetry, horizontal reflectional symmetry or rotational symmetry). Behavioural studies suggest symmetry becomes increasingly salient when the exemplars update rapidly. In line with this, Experiment 1 (N = 48) found that SPN amplitude increased when three different reflectional symmetry patterns were presented sequentially. We call this effect ‘SPN priming’. We then exploited SPN priming to investigate independence of different symmetry representations. SPN priming did not survive changes in retinal location (Experiment 2, N = 48) or non-orthogonal changes in axis orientation (Experiment 3, N = 48). However, SPN priming transferred between vertical and horizontal axis orientations (Experiment 4, N = 48) and between reflectional and rotational symmetry (Experiment 5, N = 48). SPN priming is interesting in itself, and a useful new method for identifying functional boundaries of the symmetry response. We conclude that visual regularities at different retinal locations are coded independently. However, there is some overlap between different regularities presented at the same retinal location.


2020 ◽  
Author(s):  
Emiel Cracco ◽  
Haeeun Lee ◽  
Goedele van Belle ◽  
Lisa Quenon ◽  
Patrick Haggard ◽  
...  

AbstractHumans and other animals have evolved to act in groups, but how does the brain distinguish multiple people moving in group from multiple people moving independently? Across three experiments, we test whether biological motion perception depends on the spatiotemporal relationships among people moving together. In Experiment 1, we apply EEG frequency tagging to apparent biological motion and show that fluently ordered sequences of body postures drive brain activity at three hierarchical levels of biological motion processing: image, body sequence, and movement. We then show that movement-, but not body- or image-related brain responses are enhanced when observing four agents moving in synchrony. Neural entrainment was strongest for fluently moving synchronous groups (Experiment 2), displayed in upright orientation (Experiment 3). Our findings show that the brain preferentially entrains to the collective movement of human agents, deploying perceptual organization principles of synchrony and common fate for the purpose of social perception.


2020 ◽  
Author(s):  
Alexis David James Makin ◽  
John Tyson-Carr ◽  
Marco Bertamini ◽  
Yiovanna Derpsch ◽  
Giulia Rampone

An ERP component called the Sustained Posterior Negativity (SPN) indexes the brain response to regularity in visual patterns (e.g. vertical reflectional symmetry, horizontal reflectional symmetry or rotational symmetry). However it is unclear if different regularities are coded by independent or overlapping neural populations. Previous work has successfully exploited repetition effects to assess representational independence, and we adapted this approach to investigate visual regularity. Experiment 1 (N = 48) found that SPN amplitude increased when three reflectional symmetry patterns were presented sequentially. This SPN priming effect did not survive changes in retinal location (Experiment 2, N = 48) or non-orthogonal changes in axis orientation (Experiment 3, N= 48). However, SPN priming transferred between vertical and horizontal axis orientation (Experiment 4, N= 48) and between reflectional and rotational symmetry (Experiment 5, N= 48). We conclude that visual regularities at different retinal locations are coded independently. However, there is some overlap between different regularities presented at the same retinal location.


2020 ◽  
Vol 897 ◽  
pp. 239-244
Author(s):  
Gina Fairuz ◽  
Dwinanti Rika Marthanty ◽  
Jessica Sjah

. Landuse changing along the Bidara Cina, downstream of the Ciliwung floodplain deteriorates its environmental quality. Its interactions between the building along the floodplain and the river dynamics can be simulated with Ansys Fluent as a simulation tool. This tool simulates the impact of floods could damage the buildings structure that built in flood zones but are still within the limits of habitable zone permits. Ansys Fluent modelling is using CFD parameters, by creating geometric scenarios that have been physically tested. The geometric behavior observed through the effect of large window openings (Experiment x), namely model 1 with one window and model 2 with 2 windows, then it is analysed by rotating 0°, 30°, 45°, 60° and 90° of the building (Experiment y). The outputs that will be analyse from flood induced the building are (i) flood impact force, (ii) total force of building, and (iii) reduction of flow velocity in building. From the simulation result, it is found that building geometry with larger openings is better at resisting the impact strength of floods and reducing the velocity of flood flows. Furthermore, the graph of flood impact force and the total force of orientation in model 1 and model 2 have a tedency to increase while rotation angle increasing. We conclude that based on building type (Experiment x), the model with one window is most vulnerable model and based on building orientation (Experiment y), the model with orientation 90° is the most vulnerable model, but when viewed based on facade that is exposed to the main stream there is an opening, the orientation of 60° is the most vulnerable model.


1994 ◽  
Vol 195 (1) ◽  
pp. 281-291 ◽  
Author(s):  
S Johnsen

This study tests the hypotheses that the birefringent calcite and stereom structure of the brittlestar (Ophiuroidea, Echinodermata) endoskeleton polarizes light and that certain brittlestars respond to polarized light. The first hypothesis was tested in Ophioderma brevispinum by examining ossicles from freshly killed specimens under polarized light. This analysis revealed that the lateral arm shields, oral arm shields, arm spines and aboral disk ossicles are dichroic and thus polarize light. The second hypothesis was tested in two orientation experiments under polarized light. The results from the first orientation experiment showed (1) that, under polarized light, animals oriented significantly and unimodally, (2) that, under polarized light with the e-vector perpendicular to that in 1, animals oriented significantly and unimodally to within 18 degrees of the bearing of the animals in 1, and (3) that, under unpolarized light, animals did not orient significantly. The results from the second orientation experiment showed that, under polarized light, animals oriented significantly and unimodally to within 17 degrees of an individual preference previously established under polarized light; but under unpolarized light, animals did not orient significantly to an individual preference established under polarized light. Thus, O. brevispinum orients under polarized, but not unpolarized, light. The unimodal orientation and lack of consistent alignment with the e-vector suggest that polarized light is not used as a directional cue but instead as a signal to sustain oriented behavior. The dichroism of the ossicles of O. brevispinum and the animal's capacity (though eyeless) to discriminate between polarized and unpolarized light suggest that the mechanism of polarization sensitivity may rely on polarizing filters built from the animal's skeleton.


1994 ◽  
Vol 26 (1) ◽  
pp. 23-45 ◽  
Author(s):  
P. A. Huxley ◽  
A. Pinney ◽  
E. Akunda ◽  
P. Muraya

1992 ◽  
Vol 44 (3) ◽  
pp. 475-508 ◽  
Author(s):  
Johan Wagemans ◽  
Luc Van Gool ◽  
Géry d'Ydewalle

In previous research on symmetry detection, factors contributing to orientational effects (axis and virtual lines connecting symmetrically positioned dots) and component processes (axis selection and pointwise evaluation) have always been confounded. The reason is the restriction to bilateral symmetry (BS), with pointwise correspondences being orthogonal to the axis of symmetry. In our experiments, subjects had to discriminate random dot patterns from symmetries defined by combining 12 axis orientations (every 15°) with seven reflection angles (0°, yielding BS, and three clockwise and counterclockwise 15° steps, yielding skewed symmetry, SS). In Experiment 1, with completely randomized trial order, a significant interaction between axis and skewing angle was obtained, indicating that classically observed orientational effects are restricted to BS and that the orientation of the pointwise correspondences is important. These basic findings were replicated in three subsequent experiments, which differed in that they used blocks containing patterns with the same axis (Experiment 2), virtual lines orientation (Experiment 3), or their combination (Experiment 4). Based on a comparison between the results obtained by these manipulations, we suggest a possible reason for the failure of preattentive symmetry detection in the case of dot patterns with SS.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document