angry face
Recently Published Documents


TOTAL DOCUMENTS

50
(FIVE YEARS 15)

H-INDEX

9
(FIVE YEARS 1)

2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
◽  
Lisa Michelle Hunkin

<p>Healthy individuals show an attentional bias toward threat, and this bias is exaggerated in anxious individuals. Recent studies have shown that training anxious individuals to attend to neutral information can reduce their threat bias and anxiety levels. This training is called attentional bias modification (ABM). However, despite the large literature on ABM, it is still unclear how ABM achieves its effects. Two mechanisms – facilitated engagement with threat, and delayed disengagement from threat – are thought to be involved in the threat bias. In this thesis, I investigated the effects of ABM on engagement with angry faces. First, in Experiment 1 I developed an ABM task to train healthy individuals to attend to either angry or neutral faces. Participants completed a dot-probe task in which they saw two faces – one angry and one neutral – followed by a target that appeared more often in the location of either the angry or neutral face (depending on their respective training condition). Experiment 1 was successful at inducing a bias. Next, Experiment 2 used this task to investigate the effects of ABM on event-related potentials before, during, and after training. The N2pc component, which provides a measure of attentional engagement, was used to investigate changes in engagement with angry and neutral faces as a function of training. Consistent with previous studies, there was an overall N2pc for the angry face, indicating that participants were engaging their attention with the angry face. However, the N2pc was not affected by training, even though participants were moving their eyes in the training-congruent direction during training, indicating sensitivity to the training contingency. These results suggest that ABM does not affect attentional engagement with threat stimuli. Rather, it is likely that an improved ability to disengage attention from threat stimuli underlies ABM’s training effects.</p>


2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
◽  
Lisa Michelle Hunkin

<p>Healthy individuals show an attentional bias toward threat, and this bias is exaggerated in anxious individuals. Recent studies have shown that training anxious individuals to attend to neutral information can reduce their threat bias and anxiety levels. This training is called attentional bias modification (ABM). However, despite the large literature on ABM, it is still unclear how ABM achieves its effects. Two mechanisms – facilitated engagement with threat, and delayed disengagement from threat – are thought to be involved in the threat bias. In this thesis, I investigated the effects of ABM on engagement with angry faces. First, in Experiment 1 I developed an ABM task to train healthy individuals to attend to either angry or neutral faces. Participants completed a dot-probe task in which they saw two faces – one angry and one neutral – followed by a target that appeared more often in the location of either the angry or neutral face (depending on their respective training condition). Experiment 1 was successful at inducing a bias. Next, Experiment 2 used this task to investigate the effects of ABM on event-related potentials before, during, and after training. The N2pc component, which provides a measure of attentional engagement, was used to investigate changes in engagement with angry and neutral faces as a function of training. Consistent with previous studies, there was an overall N2pc for the angry face, indicating that participants were engaging their attention with the angry face. However, the N2pc was not affected by training, even though participants were moving their eyes in the training-congruent direction during training, indicating sensitivity to the training contingency. These results suggest that ABM does not affect attentional engagement with threat stimuli. Rather, it is likely that an improved ability to disengage attention from threat stimuli underlies ABM’s training effects.</p>


2021 ◽  
Vol 12 ◽  
Author(s):  
Joshua Juvrud ◽  
Sara A. Haas ◽  
Nathan A. Fox ◽  
Gustaf Gredebäck

Development of selective attention during the first year of life is critical to cognitive and socio-emotional skills. It is also a period that the average child’s interactions with their mother dominate their social environment. This study examined how maternal negative affect and an emotion face prime (mother/stranger) jointly effect selective visual attention. Results from linear mixed-effects modeling showed that 9-month olds (N=70) were faster to find a visual search target after viewing a fearful face (regardless of familiarity) or their mother’s angry face. For mothers with high negative affect, infants’ attention was further impacted by fearful faces, resulting in faster search times. Face emotion interacted with mother’s negative affect, demonstrating a capacity to influence what infants attend in their environment.


2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
Alicia Vallorani ◽  
Kelley Gunther ◽  
Berenice Anaya ◽  
Jessica L. Burris ◽  
Andy Peter Field ◽  
...  

Introduction: Patterns of affect-biased attention are related to anxiety and anxiety risk. However, little is known regarding how affect-biased attention develops. Recent work suggests relations with both infant temperamental negative affect and maternal anxiety. The current paper examines potential bi-directional relations between infant attention, infant negative affect, and maternal anxiety to better understand a developmental process that may precede the emergence of anxiety. Method: Infant-mother pairs (N = 333) participated in a multi-site, longitudinal study providing eye-tracking and questionnaire data when infants were 4-, 8-, 12-, 18- and 24-months. A random intercepts cross-lag panel model assessed bi-directional relations between infant attention, infant negative affect and maternal anxiety.Results: Within-person deviations in maternal anxiety were prospectively, negatively related to within-person deviations in infant attention to angry face configurations at every assessment and within-person deviations in infant attention to happy face configurations at the final two assessments. Additionally, within-person deviations in infant negative affect were prospectively, positively related to within-person deviations in infant attention to angry face configurations at 12- and 18-months. Consistent bi-directional relations were not found.Conclusion: Our results suggest that infants do not display a stable bias to threat in the first 24 months of life. Rather, individual differences, in this case maternal anxiety and infant negative affect, shape patterns of attention biases over time. The current results provide an initial understanding of bi-directional relations in affect-biased attention development.


2021 ◽  
pp. 1-14
Author(s):  
Resh S. Gupta ◽  
Autumn Kujawa ◽  
David R. Vago

Abstract. Threat-related attention bias is thought to contribute to the development and maintenance of anxiety disorders. Dot-probe studies using event-related potentials (ERPs) have indicated that several early ERP components are modulated by threatening and emotional stimuli in anxious populations, suggesting enhanced allocation of attention to threat and emotion at earlier stages of processing. However, ERP components selected for examination and analysis in these studies vary widely and remain inconsistent. The present study used temporospatial principal component analysis (PCA) to systematically identify ERP components elicited to face pair cues and probes in a dot-probe task in anxious adults. Cue-locked components sensitive to emotion included an early occipital C1 component enhanced for happy versus angry face pair cues and an early parieto-occipital P1 component enhanced for happy versus angry face pair cues. Probe-locked components sensitive to congruency included a parieto-occipital P2 component enhanced for incongruent probes (probes replacing neutral faces) versus congruent probes (probes replacing emotional faces). Split-half correlations indicated that the mean value around the PCA-derived peaks was reliably measured in the ERP waveforms. These results highlight promising neurophysiological markers for attentional bias research that can be extended to designs comparing anxious and healthy comparison groups. Results from a secondary exploratory PCA analysis investigating the effects of emotional face position and analyses on behavioral reaction time data are also presented.


2021 ◽  
Vol 12 ◽  
Author(s):  
Kristina M. Hengen ◽  
Georg W. Alpers

Stress and anxiety can both influence risk-taking in decision-making. While stress typically increases risk-taking, anxiety often leads to risk-averse choices. Few studies have examined both stress and anxiety in a single paradigm to assess risk-averse choices. We therefore set out to examine emotional decision-making under stress in socially anxious participants. In our study, individuals (N = 87) high or low in social anxiety completed an expanded variation of the Balloon Analogue Risk Task (BART). While inflating a balloon to a larger degree is rewarded, a possible explosion leads to (a) a loss of money and (b) it is followed by an emotional picture (i.e., a calm vs. an angry face). To induce stress before this task, participants were told that they would have to deliver a speech. We operationalized risk-taking by the number of pumps during inflation and its functionality by the amount of monetary gain. In addition, response times were recorded as an index of decisional conflict. Without the stressor, high socially anxious compared to low socially anxious participants did not differ in any of the dependent variables. However, under stress, the low socially anxious group took more risk and earned more money, while high socially anxious individuals remained more cautious and did not change their risk-taking under social stress. Overall, high socially anxious individuals made their decisions more hesitantly compared to low socially anxious individuals. Unexpectedly, there were no main effects or interactions with the valence of the emotional faces. This data shows that stress affects socially anxious individuals differently: in low socially anxious individuals stress fosters risk-taking, whereas high socially anxious individuals did not alter their behavior and remained risk-averse. The novel eBART is a promising research tool to examine the specific factors that influence decision-making.


2021 ◽  
Author(s):  
AmirHussein Abdolalizadeh ◽  
Kamyar Moradi ◽  
Mohammad Amin Dabbagh Ohadi ◽  
FatemehSadat Mirfazeli ◽  
Reza Rajimehr

AbstractBackgroundConduct Disorder (CD) is defined as aggressive, antisocial, and rule-breaking behavior during childhood. Despite being the major risk factor for developing an antisocial personality disorder, nearly half the patients return to a nearly normal and healthy status. We aimed to identify psychiatric, emotional, and brain volumetric footprints of childhood CD in healthy young adults with a prior history of CD.MethodsWe recruited 156 healthy young adults from the Human Connectome Project: 78 with a prior history of CD (CC) and 78 age-, gender-, and education-matched subjects with no history of CD (HC). Psychiatric, emotional, and personality assessments were based on the Adult Self Report, NIH toolbox emotion battery and Penn emotion recognition task, and NEO-Five Factor Inventory. We acquired MRI volumetric measures and hippocampal and amygdala segmentation values using FreeSurfer. Between-group differences and associations between the assessments and the hippocampal or amygdala nuclei were assessed statistically.ResultsAfter Benjamini-Hochberg correction, we found higher anger aggression, antisocial personality problems, aggressive and rule-breaking behaviors, externalizing, and lower agreeableness in the CC group. No differences between volumetric measures were noted. Interestingly, only in the CC group, better angry face recognition was associated with larger volumes of several hippocampal nuclei.DiscussionDespite having no notable MRI volumetric differences, healthy young adults with a prior history of CD still exhibit some forms of antisocial-like behavior, without evidence of emotional recognition disturbances. Moreover, hippocampal-related learning may provide a compensatory mechanism for angry face recognition deficits in CD, playing a potential protective role.


2021 ◽  
pp. 1-16
Author(s):  
Arianna M. Gard ◽  
Tyler C. Hein ◽  
Colter Mitchell ◽  
Jeanne Brooks-Gunn ◽  
Sarah S. McLanahan ◽  
...  

Abstract Childhood adversity is thought to undermine youth socioemotional development via altered neural function within regions that support emotion processing. These effects are hypothesized to be developmentally specific, with adversity in early childhood sculpting subcortical structures (e.g., amygdala) and adversity during adolescence impacting later-developing structures (e.g., prefrontal cortex; PFC). However, little work has tested these theories directly in humans. Using prospectively collected longitudinal data from the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study (FFCWS) (N = 4,144) and neuroimaging data from a subsample of families recruited in adolescence (N = 162), the current study investigated the trajectory of harsh parenting across childhood (i.e., ages 3 to 9) and how initial levels versus changes in harsh parenting across childhood were associated with corticolimbic activation and connectivity during socioemotional processing. Harsh parenting in early childhood (indexed by the intercept term from a linear growth curve model) was associated with less amygdala, but not PFC, reactivity to angry facial expressions. In contrast, change in harsh parenting across childhood (indexed by the slope term) was associated with less PFC, but not amygdala, activation to angry faces. Increases in, but not initial levels of, harsh parenting were also associated with stronger positive amygdala–PFC connectivity during angry face processing.


2020 ◽  
Author(s):  
Arianna M Gard ◽  
Tyler C. Hein ◽  
Colter Mitchell ◽  
Sara McLanahan ◽  
Luke Williamson Hyde

Manuscript in press at Development &amp; Psychopathology (Submitted 8/17/2020, Accepted 11/08/2020). Childhood adversity is thought to undermine youth socioemotional development via altered neural function within regions that support emotion processing. These effects are hypothesized to be developmentally specific, with adversity in early childhood sculpting subcortical structures (e.g., amygdala) and adversity during adolescence impacting later-developing structures (e.g., prefrontal cortex; PFC). However, little work has tested these theories directly in humans. Using prospectively-collected longitudinal data from the Fragile Families and Child Wellbeing Study (N = 4,144) and neuroimaging data from a subsample of families recruited in adolescence (N = 162), the current study investigated the trajectory of harsh parenting across childhood (i.e., ages 3 to 9) and how initial levels versus changes in harsh parenting across childhood were associated with corticolimbic activation and connectivity during socioemotional processing. Harsh parenting in early childhood (indexed by the intercept term from a linear growth curve model) was associated with less amygdala, but not PFC, reactivity to angry facial expressions. In contrast, change in harsh parenting across childhood (indexed by the slope term) was associated with less PFC, but not amygdala, activation to angry faces. Increases in, but not initial levels of, harsh parenting were also associated with stronger positive amygdala-PFC connectivity during angry face processing


2020 ◽  
Vol 17 (1) ◽  
pp. 1-18
Author(s):  
Melissa Adler

Studies show that emotion enhances memory for individual items but weakens memory for associations between items (Bisby & Burgess, 2014). One explanation for this associative memory impairment is that emotional stimuli capture attention, causing enhanced encoding of the emotional item but reduced encoding of the surrounding environment (Schupp, Junghöfer, Weike, & Hamm, 2003). This explanation generates the prediction that emotional information always impairs associative memory. Alternatively, it may be that emotion orients attention towards threats in the environment, suggesting that emotions’ effects on associative memory may differ depending on where they indicate a threat may be coming from (Öhman, Flykt, & Esteves, 2001). For example, seeing an angry face constitutes a direct threat. The angry face itself potentially captures attention and thereby reduces memory for its associated information. In contrast, seeing a fearful face indicates a threat elsewhere in the environment. Therefore, the fearful face may redirect attention towards the surroundings and thus enhance encoding of the associated information. To adjudicate between these hypotheses, subjects studied sets of three images, consisting of two objects and a face with either a neutral, angry, or fearful expression. Subjects were later tested on their memory for the associations between the three items. Supporting the first hypothesis, memory for both angry and fearful associations was worse than memory for neutral associations. Contrary to the second hypothesis, there were no differences in memory for angry versus fearful associations. Thus, emotional information itself seems to capture attention, weakening memory for related information.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document