Sustained Attention to Science: A Tribute to the Life and Scholarship of Joel Warm

Author(s):  
P. A. Hancock ◽  
James L. Szalma

Objective: To provide an evaluative synthesis of the life and scientific contributions of the late Joel Warm. Background: As the doyen of vigilance research, Joel Warm expanded our understanding and horizons concerning this critical response capacity. However, he also made widespread and profound contributions to many other areas of perception and applied psychology, as we elucidate here. Method: Using archival sources, personal histories, and analysis of extant literature documenting Warm’s own productivity, we articulate his life in science. Results: Our synthesis illustrates the continued, broad, influential, and expanding impact that one individual can exert on diverse fields of study. Whole bodies of understanding of human behavior have been illuminated by his exemplary career. Application By understanding his path to success in applied experimental psychology, we anticipate that others will be motivated, inspired, and guided to replicate and even outstrip a lifetime of such seminal and influential contributions. The presence of individuals such as Warm serves as a primary motive in enhancing Humans Factors/Ergonomics Science.

2012 ◽  
Vol 17 (4) ◽  
pp. 320-325 ◽  
Author(s):  
Serge Nicolas ◽  
Zachary Levine

Though Alfred Binet was a prolific writer, many of his 1893–1903 works are not well known. This is partly due to a lack of English translations of the many important papers and books that he and his collaborators created during this period. Binet’s insights into intelligence testing are widely celebrated, but the centennial of his death provides an occasion to reexamine his other psychological examinations. His studies included many diverse aspects of mental life, including memory research and the science of testimony. Indeed, Binet was a pioneer of psychology and produced important research on cognitive and experimental psychology, developmental psychology, social psychology, and applied psychology. This paper seeks to elucidate these aspects of his work.


Author(s):  
Brian H. Bornstein ◽  
Jeffrey S. Neuschatz

Psychology was a relatively young science when Münsterberg published On the Witness Stand, and efforts to apply psychological principles to legal issues were not much older. Law and psychology take very different epistemological approaches, and the threads of the two disciplines have come together and diverged over the years. This chapter includes a brief biography of Münsterberg and a summary of other contemporary work that addressed legal issues. The chapter also introduces important contrasts that have been central to the field since Münsterberg’s day. Of particular note, Münsterberg conducted basic psychological research but is also considered a pioneer in the field of applied psychology, albeit at times an ambivalent one; and current tensions between clinical and experimental psychology date back to his day and the early years of the American Psychological Association.


Author(s):  
Elizabeth Hellmuth Margulis

Music can seem to be the human behavior that is least susceptible to explanation, but a long history exists of applying various frameworks to try to understand it. The cognitive science of music integrates ideas from philosophy, music theory, experimental psychology, neuroscience, anthropology, and computer modeling to answer questions about music’s role in people’s lives. The art of music psychology is to bring rigorous scientific methodologies to questions about the human musical capacity while applying sophisticated humanistic approaches to framing and interpreting the science.


Author(s):  
Javier Bandrés

In the history of Spanish psychology in the 19th century, three stages can be distinguished. An eclectic first stage was defined by the coexistence of currents such as spiritualism, sensism, ideology, and common-sense realism. Jaime Balmes was the most prominent and original author, integrating empiricism and associationism in the Spanish tradition of common-sense philosophy. The second stage was characterized by the influence of Krausism, a version of German rationalist pantheism imported by Julián Sanz del Río, that reached great acceptance during the 1860s and 1870s among intellectuals opposed to traditional Catholicism. The third stage began in the late 1870s: the reception, adaptation, development, and debate of the “new psychology” flowing from Germany, Great Britain, and France. A group of neo-Kantian intellectuals led by Cuban José del Perojo, a disciple of Kuno Fischer, introduced and popularized experimental psychology and comparative psychology in Spain. His project was vigorously seconded in Cuba by Enrique José Varona, author of the first Spanish manual of experimental psychology. In this path, the Marxist psychiatrist and intellectual Jaime Vera promoted in Madrid a materialistic view of psychology, and his colleague and friend Luis Simarro won the first university chair of Experimental Psychology, fostering a school of psychologists oriented toward experimental science. In turn, the publication in 1879 of the papal encyclical Aeterni Patris stimulated the development of a Spanish neoscholastic scientific psychology, developed under the influence of Cardinal Mercier of the Catholic University of Louvain. Authors such as Zeferino González, Marcelino Arnáiz, and Alberto Gómez Izquierdo broke with the anti-modern tradition of the Spanish Church and developed an experimental psychology within the Aristotelian-Thomistic framework. In the first three decades of the 20th century, applied psychology expanded radically, linked to a period of strong socioeconomic growth. Abnormal and educational psychology developed vigorously, and Spanish psychotechnics, led by José Germain in Madrid and Emilio Mira in Barcelona, was at the forefront of European science. In 1936 the Spanish Civil War imposed a bloody parenthesis to the economic and scientific development of the country. In the postwar period, the psychiatrist Antonio Vallejo-Nágera and his group tried to manipulate psychological research to legitimize some of general Franco's policies. Simultaneously, two neoscholastic scholars, Manuel Barbado and Juan Zaragüeta, supervised the recovery and scientific development of Spanish psychology through institutions such as the Department of Experimental Psychology of the Higher Council for Scientific Research, the National Institute of Psychotechnics, and the School of Applied Psychology and Psychotechnics of the University of Madrid. José Germain was chosen to direct and guide these projects, and a new generation of academic psychologists was formed: Mariano Yela, José Luis Pinillos, and Miguel Siguán, among others. The economic expansion of the 1960s and 1970s and the end of Franco’s dictatorship produced a huge development of academic and professional psychology, with Spanish psychology becoming positively integrated into Western science. On the other side of the Atlantic, the psychology of liberation developed by Ignacio Martín-Baró in El Salvador promoted the theoretical and methodological renewal of Latin American psychology.


1975 ◽  
Vol 3 (2) ◽  
pp. 28-30
Author(s):  
R. S. Hallam

About 10 years ago, experimental psychology seemed to have little to offer for the analysis and solution of mental health problems. We all remember the joke about the psychologist searching under the lamp-post where the light was strongest. It is not repeated as often nowadays - and with good reason. Concepts from experimental psychology have come to play an increasingly central role in psychiatry. The following is an analysis of the percentage of articles in the British Journal of Psychiatry which were not concerned with psychiatric diagnosis, psychoanalysis, dynamic psychotherapy, physical therapies, neurology, epidemiology, genetics, prognosis and aetiology of syndromes, in the years 1966, 1970, 1973. Psychometric and psychophysiological studies were also excluded if they related to differential diagnosis of psychiatric states. The figures were 11, 13 and 23% for the three years, suggesting a slowly accelerating utilization of concepts derived from experimental psychology. If the rate of growth continues, models derived from experimental psychology will be dominant by 1980.


2011 ◽  
Vol 32 (3) ◽  
pp. 117-128 ◽  
Author(s):  
Stefan Krumm ◽  
Lothar Schmidt-Atzert ◽  
Maren Bracht ◽  
Lisa Ochs

Coordination is a well-known concept in experimental psychology. The coordination of action patterns was also proposed as a key determinant in paper-pencil sustained attention/concentration tests. To date, however, no studies have tried to prove the relevance of coordination ability in sustained attention tests. We conducted two studies (n = 199 and n = 132) to experimentally vary a prominent sustained attention tests: the Digit-Symbol Substitution Test. Using an approach derived from multitasking research, we extracted a residual that presumably comprised variance due to coordination. This presumption was confirmed in Study 1: The coordination ability was stable, correlated significantly with other paper-pencil sustained attention tests, and was also related to the executive function switching. In Study 2, a different approach led to similar results: We could show that practice gains can be attributed to an improved ability to coordinate basic action patterns. It is concluded that coordination is a core element of sustained attention/concentration.


1973 ◽  
Vol 1 (1) ◽  
pp. 19-27 ◽  
Author(s):  
Gary R. Collins

Modern psychology has been shaped by three major forces: Clinical psychology, which grew out of Freudian psychoanalysis; experimental psychology, which has been modeled after Watson's behaviorism; and more recently, the humanistic psychology of Maslow and others. Each of these forces has sought to understand and mold human behavior, but each has had limited success; many psychologists today are trying to find ways to make psychology more effective. The present paper suggests that psychology's problems can be traced to an inadequate set of foundational presuppositions. It is argued that if we are to keep from disintegrating and fading into possible insignificance, psychology must re-examine its underlying presuppositions and must change these to conform to principles that are revealed in the Bible. This may result in a significant change in the future methods, content, and direction of psychology.


1997 ◽  
Vol 9 (2) ◽  
pp. 189-191 ◽  
Author(s):  
DANTE CICCHETTI ◽  
JOHN E. RICHTERS

The interdisciplinary science of developmental psychopathology has evolved from its historical roots in a variety of fields including: sociology; epidemiology; embryology; the neurosciences and psychobiology; psychoanalysis; clinical, developmental, and experimental psychology; and psychiatry (Cicchetti, 1990) into an increasingly mature integrative framework within which the contributions of these heretofore largely separate disciplines could be fully realized within the broader context of understanding individual development and functioning, both normal and abnormal (see chapters in Cicchetti & Cohen, 1995a, 1995b; see also Cicchetti & Toth, in press). In one of the early statements concerning the goals of this field, Cicchetti (1990) asserted that: “Developmental psychopathology ... should bridge fields of study, span the life cycle, and aid in the discovery of important new truths about the processes underlying adaptation and maladaptation, as well as the best means of preventing or ameliorating psychopathology. Moreover, this discipline should contribute greatly to reducing the dualisms that exist between the clinical study of and research into childhood and adult disorders, between the behavioral and biological sciences, between developmental psychology and psychopathology, and between basic and applied science” (p. 20).


2019 ◽  
Vol 34 (s1) ◽  
pp. s141-s141
Author(s):  
Elisa N. Saleme ◽  
Luz A. De la Sierra ◽  
Jose L. Kramis ◽  
Pedro Arguelles ◽  
Hector Montiel

Introduction:Mexico has suffered multiple social and natural events that tested its response capacity. Hospital units of the third level of care are an axis of response and a central reference. Guaranteeing their integral and organized response promotes risk prevention and mitigation strategy in emergencies and disasters.Aim:To analyze the national and international regulations and the existing documents about emergency and disasters related to a hospital with the identification of the critical actors in the response.Methods:This research consists of a cross-sectional and descriptive study with a mixed methodology (qualitative and quantitative), that generates a protocol for response in a third level care hospital. Quantitative analysis was carried out using central tendency measurements based on a surveys (training, knowledge) performed in the hospital services that provide a critical response with the ED in emergencies or disasters (ED, ICU, Supplies, Nursing, Operating Room, Security, Hospital Admission, Crisis Committee). In the quantitative analysis, the staff were interviewed about their experience in responding to previous events (to the same critical services), recognizing importance and points of improvement with a discourse analysis methodology.Results:With the information collected and based on the protocols of Safe Hospital program (PAHO/WHO) we generated a protocol organized by the ED that involves massive victims.Discussion:Regulations oblige hospital units to have protocols of action in critical situations linked to Safe Hospital program, so it is a great tool for planning. All the surveyed personnel consider that it is important to have a plan that allows for immediate steps to ensure quality and timely patient care, considering it an ethical and social obligation. Analysis suggests that continuous training and the contribution of an operational plan per service provide security and better prognosis to the victims. The protocol includes all critical response services with a clinical practice guide.


2019 ◽  
Vol 8 (4) ◽  
pp. 106-114
Author(s):  
Y.A. Kochetova ◽  
M.V. Klimakova

The problem of diagnostics of emotional intelligence is important both for applied psychology and for theoretical research in this field of knowledge. The purpose of the article is to review the methods of diagnosing emotional intelligence. The diagnostic toolkit for research of the dynamics of emotional intelligence depends on the explanatory model of emotional intelligence. At the same time, within the framework of one methodical approach, both diagnostic tools and the components of emotional intelligence measured by them are different. Based on these features of the problem one can put forward the principles of designing the diagnostic techniques. Summarizing these features and principles, we can figuratively divide diagnostic methodical material into groups as follows: describing the specific situation of the tasks; task wording that excludes ambiguous understanding of them; the wording of the assignments to avoid conscious control and socially desirable responses; highlighting objective criteria for assessing responses; measuring not personality traits, but abilities; possibility to predict human behavior in areas associated with emotional manifestations.


Sign in / Sign up

Export Citation Format

Share Document